.

Saturday, 9 March 2019

Neuropsychology of Language

The neuropsychological courtes are gradually leading to primal discoveries about many aspects of chief purpose, and linguistic process is no exception. Progress has surely been made in identifying the structure and bring of wording(s), its universal takes, its acquisition and so on, unless, until recently, this work has tended to ignore pathologies of language. More recently, neuropsychologists submit begun to draw parallels between aphasic disorders and disruption to special linguistic processes.This work provides evidence of a picture dissociation between semantic and syntactic processes, and illustrates clearly that no whiz creative thinker language centre exists. The development of enquiry tools such as the Wada test, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging procedures, has enabled researchers to examine language function in the brains of normal individuals.This work considers the miscellaneous ways that scientists confound examined lateralisation, a nd the conclusions that they yield drawn from their research. The work supports the view that language is mediated by a series of interconnected cortical regions in the left cerebral hemisphere, much as the nineteenth century neurologists proposed. In addition, this work considers recent explorations of language functions in the brain using neurophysiological techniques.At out pock glance, the two cortical hemispheres look rather like mirror images of each otherwise. The brain, like other components of the nervous system, is superficially symmetrical along the midline, but closer reexamination reveals many differences in structure, and behavioural studies suggest differences in function too. The fountain for these so-called asymmetries is unclear, although they are widely assumed to depend on the motion of genes. Some writers have suggested that they are particularly linked to the development in humans of a sophisticated language system (Crow, 1998). Others have argued that th e asymmetries predated the show of language and are related to tool use and hand preference.scientific interest in language dates back to the earliest attempts by researchers to rent the brain in a systematic way, with the work of Dax, Broca and Wernicke in the 19th century. Since then, interest in all aspects of language has intensified to the point where its psychological study (psycholinguistics) is now recognised as a discipline in its own right. In 1874 Karl Wernicke describe two patients who had a quite contrasting type of language disorder. Their speech was fluent but incomprehensible and they in like manner had pro put up delicateies understanding spoken language.Wernicke later examined the brain of one of these patients and found damage in the posterior part of the superior temporal gyrus on the left. At the same time as characterising this second cook of language disorder, which we now call Wernickes aphasia, Wernicke developed a theory of how the various brain reg ions with responsibility for receptive and expressive language function interact. His ideas were taken up and developed by Lichtheim and later, by Geschwind.In Brocas aphasia, as with close neurological conditions, impairment is a matter of degree, but the core feature is a marked difficulty in producing coherent speech (hence the secondary names of expressive or non-fluent aphasia). Brocas aphasics can use well-practised expressions without obvious difficulty, and they may to a fault be able to sing a well-known song faultlessly.These abilities question that the problem is not related to the mechanics of moving the muscles that are concerned with speech. Wernickes first patient had difficulty in understanding speech yet could verbalise fluently, although what he said usually did not make much sense. This form of aphasia clearly differed in several respects from that described by Broca. The problems for Wernickes patient were related to comprehension and meaningful widening ra ther than the agrammatical and telegraphic output slangn in Brocas patients.Brocas and Wernickes work generated considerable interest among fellow researchers. In 1885, Lichtheim proposed what has discern to be known as the connectionist model of language to explain the various forms of aphasia (seven in all) that had, by then, been characterised. Incidentally, the term connectionist implies that different brain centres are interconnected, and that impaired language function may result either from damage to one of the centres or to the path-In Lichtheims model, Brocas and Wernickes areas formed two points of a triangle (Franklin 2003).The third point delineate a concept centre where word meanings were stored and where auditory comprehension thus occurred. severally point was interconnected, so that damage, either to one of the centres (points), or to any of the pathways connecting them would attain some form of aphasia. Lichtheims model explained many of the peculiarities of dif ferent forms of aphasia, and became, for a time, the preponderant model of how the brain manages language comprehension and production.Three new lines of doubt the cognitive neuropsychology approach, the functional neuro-imaging research of Petersen, Raichle and colleagues, and the neuroanatomical work of Dronkers and colleagues have prompted new ideas about the networks of brain regions that mediate language. Researchers in the newly acclivitous field of developmental cognitive neuroscience seek to understand how postnatal brain development relates to changes in perceptual, cognitive, and social abilities in infants and children (Johnson 2005).The cognitive neuropsychological approach has underlined the subtle differences in cognitive processes that may give rise to specific language disorders. The functional imaging research has identified a wider present of left brain (and some right brain) regions that are clearly prompt as numbers undertake language tasks. The emerging view from these diverse research approaches is that language is a far more complex and sophisticated acquisition than was once thought.A universal design feature of languages is that their meaning-bearing forms are dual-lane into two different subsystems, the open-class, or lexical, and the closed-class, or grammatical (Johnson 1997). Open classes have many members and can readily add many more. They commonly take on (the roots of) nouns, verbs, and adjectives.Closed classes have relatively few members and are difficult to augment. They include such bound forms as inflections (say, those appearing on a verb) and such free forms as prepositions, conjunctions, and determiners. In addition to such unresolved closed classes, there are implicit closed classes such as the set of grammatical categories that appear in a language (say, nounhood, verbhood, etc., per se), and the set of grammatical relations that appear in a language (say, subject status, direct object status, etc.).The wor k supports a model of hemispheric speciality in humans. While it would be an oversimplification to call the left hemisphere the language hemisphere and the right hemisphere the spatial (or non-language) hemisphere, it is easy to see why earlier researchers jumped to this conclusion. Whether this is because the left hemisphere is preordained for language, or because it is innately part at analytic and sequential processing, is currently a matter of debate.The uncorrupted neurological approach to understanding the role of the brain in language relied on case studies of people with localised damage, usually to the left hemisphere. Broca and Wernicke described differing forms of aphasia, the prominent features of the former being non-fluent agrammatical speech, and those of the latter being fluent but usually unintelligible speech. Their work led to the development of Lichtheims connectionist model of language, which underline both localisation of function and the connections betwee n functional areas.BibliographyBrook, A. & Atkins K. (2005). Cognition and the brain the philosophy and neuroscience movement. Cambridge, NY Cambridge University Press.Crain, W. (1992). Theories of Development Concepts and applications. Englewood Cliffs Prentice Hall.Crow, T.J. (1998). Nuclear schizophrenic symptoms as a window on the relationship between thought and speech. British ledger of Psychiatry, 173, 303-309.Franklin, Ronald D. (2003). Prediction in Forensic and Neuropsychology Sound Statistical Practices. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Mahwah, NJ.Johnson, M. H. (1997). developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford Blackwell Publishers Ltd.Johnson, M. H. (2005) Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Blackwell, Oxford, 2nd Ed.Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I.Q. (1996). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, quaternary edition, New York Freeman and Co.Maruish, Mark and E. Moses, Jr. (1997). Clinical Neuropsychology suppositional Foundations for Practitioners. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates M ahwah, NJ.Loring, D.W. (1999). INS Dictionary of Neuropsychology. Oxford Oxford University Press.Stirling, J. (2002). Introducing Neuropsychology. Psychology Press New York.

No comments:

Post a Comment